Law in Contemporary Society

Are We Losing Our Minds? : Studying Memory

-- By AnneFox - 28 Apr 2012

The first emotion I remember feeling in life was fear. The aching sound of the piano strings as my mother’s fingers floated from chord to chord while I lazed on the sofa. That was the moment when I realized exactly what death meant—that someday when I drifted off to sleep, as I was at that instant, I would not wake. More than seventeen years later I still recoil when I hear Layla's piano coda.

Ask me to restate Justice O’Connor’s concurrence in Lawrence v. Texas and my description will not be so vivid, even though I just read over the case this afternoon. It’s easy for us to focus on not only fear, but emotions in general. We’re easily compelled by episodic memory (life events) as well, though much less so by semantic memory (general knowledge). I’ve found this issue of memory and recollection especially problematic in law school where I’m expected to jam my brain full of text every day for four months and then spew it all out in a three hour exam at term’s end. The problem appears to be twofold (at the least)—a combination of lack of focus and an over-reliance on technology, and the solution might just end up being easier than my Con Law exam.

Focus Versus Distraction

Psychologists assert that memory skill is not directly affected by how much information we pump into ourselves, but rather that “the more things we have to remember that are similar to other things, the harder it is to focus on a single item.” This is certainly true in law school where every case can be distilled into facts, issue and holding. The key, perhaps, is to focus not just on what the case says or how it compares to other cases, but how you can relate it to something else you know—something that you might find more emotionally interesting.

Many people not only find it hard to focus on specifics and commit them to memory, but even to focus at all. It’s all too easy to zone out while reading, and some psychologists have devoted their entire lives to the “wandering mind” theory, where the trick is to train oneself to practice focusing by adding information to the brain in “smaller bundles” and taking the time to meditate on the distracting thoughts separately. It sounds easy—if my anxiety about my daily budget shrinking from $11 to $6 dollars is drawing my mind away from a Regulatory Takings reading, then I should just stop and plan my budget instead of fake-reading twenty pages.

Unfortunately, in today’s world, interruptions don’t only come from one’s own mind or the people in the room, but from laptops and cell phones. Studies show that students who work on several tasks simultaneously (like checking Facebook, answering texts and studying) retain far less information and do poorly in their studies. Even without an email notification or a phone beep, many modern people still constantly think about their online presence or text conversations. While I think this is a huge identity problem in it’s own right, it also has a detrimental effect on learning. Being able to completely disconnect is a virtue in being able to concentrate.

The Google Effect

Another way that technology influences our recollection abilities is through what certain psychologists term the "Google Effect." I appreciate the Internet as much as the next twenty two year old—as much as the next academic in general. It offers an infinite wealth of information at my fingertips whenever I need and has truly revolutionized the world. Unfortunately, the Internet has also completely altered the way people process information. A group of psychologists, headed by Columbia’s Betsy Sparrow, compares the individual/Internet relationship with transactive memory (“a combination of memory stores held directly by individuals”). Basically, just as we’ve known how to garner information from parents, professors or co-workers, we now turn more often to search engines. We no longer store as much direct information on our own "hard drives" (our brains), but rather we replace it with indirect knowledge of where to find the facts on the computer.

This is unsettling. I am not a robot. I would like to be able to survive someday if I were lost in the woods without a computer or an iPhone. It seems easy to place the blame on technology for people’s “stupidity.” But alas, since I am not a robot and I am not in fact controlled by the Internet, I have to recognize that I command my own knowledge and my own memory—for the most part at least.

Why Does Our Memory Matter?

As students, we learn mostly by reading and attending lectures, so whatever our memories take away is the material from which we form knowledge about a particular subject. As lawyers, we’ll learn from researching and from sapping the transactive memory of our colleagues.

If the greatest strength of a lawyer is creativity, then the second greatest strength is recollection. The more raw material we have to work with, the more pieces-parts we have to build our arguments in our imaginations. And certainly we can find all kinds of information on Westlaw or Google, but we’re more likely to understand what we’ve already retained. Moreover, law is about more than doctrine—it’s about human behavior, so perhaps our minds, if focused, are more tailored to the task anyhow.

910 Words

PS I would like to continue to edit


Very interesting post Anne. This actually reminds me of an Isaac Asimov short story that I read (or at least I think it was an Asimov story). Fittingly, I don't actually remember the name of the story or even anything about it other than the description of one character having the unique ability of knowing exactly what questions to ask machines in order to find the relevant materials. I, unlike this character, do not know the right words to search, which is why I was unable to find the story even after two hours of Googling. I did, however, find another Asimov short story, "Sucker Bait," with this revealing passage:

"'Computers are limited, captain. They have to be asked questions. What’s more the questions have to be the kind that can be put into a limited number of symbols. What’s more computers are very literal minded. They answer exactly what you ask and not what you have in mind. Sometimes it never occurs to anyone to ask just the right question or feed the computer just the right symbols, and when that happens the computer doesn’t volunteer information. 'What we need . . . what all mankind needs . . . is a computer that is nonmechanical; a computer with imagination. There’s one like that, captain.' The psychologist tapped his temple. 'In everyone, captain.'"

The problem with having an external storage device and not relying on your own memory is that an external storage device is free standing and requires purposive and conscious communication with it. The mode and language of that communication then becomes an added concern. Presumably (I don't know if this is right, so please correct me if I'm wrong) the way we access our memories is very adaptive and intensely personal. Although unconscious, we have a well understood way of communicating with our memories. With dependence on external storage devices, we must have a conscious understanding of the modes of communication with the storage device. With search engines, this often means having a conscious understanding of the most common ways certain ideas are phrased by other people. For example, right now I'm working on an appeal of a trial court's denial of a challenge to excuse a juror for cause. The cause cited isn't an express statutory provision, so it's not a question of whether the case falls into one well defined legal category or another. One side is claiming the juror was too biased based on what she said when questioned and the other side is saying that she wasn't. It's a very fact specific issue and the task becomes finding cases with similar facts. This is really hard to search for in Westlaw. A fact pattern is not something you can easily search for. What this boils down to is what one or two words do appellate judges most commonly use to describe a fact pattern like this. That word turned out to be "equivocal" - the juror was equivocal about whether she could be fair to both sides. The point is precisely what the Asimov character states - computers can only be asked a limited number of symbols and sometimes it never occurs to you what the right symbol is. The more we rely on external storage devices, the more we have to adjust ourselves to how they operate.

But unlike with our own minds, communication with external storage devices are one-sided. We type in searches until we get something we want, but the search engine (as far as I know) does not reflect on and adapt to our searches. Maybe Google does this now, but it's hard to think of a computer learning from people's searches and adjusting the results if people are unsatisfied and keep searching for the same thing. It would have to be a very sophisticated algorithm to be able to recognize this behavior well enough to adjust, let alone learn by itself without having to be manually readjusted every once in a while. Either way though, insofar as we depend on a shared external storage device, we have to develop a common mode of communication - communicating what is most commonly said will give us the best results.

I think all this conscious adjustment, learning how to best communicate with search engines, and predicting what the words most people would use is probably far less efficient than relying on our own memories that does all of these automatically and unconsciously.

Another related issue that this made me think of how this all relates to personal identity (at least metaphorically). John Locke famously proclaimed that personal identity is founded in consciousness, more specifically, “the same continued consciousness.” (See Chapter 27 “Of Identity and Diversity” http://etext.lib.virginia.edu/etcbin/toccer-new2?id=LocHuma.xml&images=images/modeng&data=/texts/english/modeng/parsed&tag=public&part=all). This basically boils down to memory because it is our memories that connect our conscious states over time. But if the persistence of memory is what defines personal identity, then what happens when our memories are more and more often stored in external machines? It seems that at least metaphorically, we become those machines. Locke’s main point of course is that identity isn’t founded in substance or body, but on consciousness, so we are not actually machines. But the more we depend on machines to house our memories, the more they and not our physical bodies are the immediate mediums of our existence. In that sense at least, we may actually become robots.

-- AlexWang - 28 Jun 2012

Alex,

Thanks for the input--sorry I can't help you remember the name of the short story, but maybe the transactive memory of the class can help you out even if Google didn't. I agree with you on everything you've mentioned and I think that this kind of disconnect between humanity and technology is something that merits more thought--especially with it comes to personal identity.

I ironically was about to post on this draft myself before you posted your comments, so I apologize if what follows doesn't directly address your points, though it still might be of interest to you/I'd like to know people's opinions on the matter.

In the time since I wrote this paper, I’ve continued researching the topic, which has led me to focus more specifically on the relationship between the creative mind and the internet. I want to keep editing my paper, but I’m not sure how to integrate all this new information in without completely rewriting the entire thing, so I thought I’d just brainstorm a little until I get more feedback. I just picked up Nicholas Carr’s 2010 book The Shallows, which is an extension of his 2009 Atlantic cover piece “Is Google Making Us Stupid?”an article I’m sure many of us have already read.

Jim Holt’s review of The Shallows is actually what convinced me to read the book itself, and it’s definitely worth a look for a summary of Carr’s option as well as some criticism. Carr’s main point is that computers and access to the internet not only change the way the human mind works, but that the changes lead to negative consequences.

Initially, I absolutely agreed. I grew up in a small town flanked by farmland and Amish communities and I always idolized the peace that came with their unadorned lifestyle. Now I take notes in class on my MacBook? air while I have four or five other applications open. Still, I’ve always preferred unhurriedly reading page to page out of a book to the rapid pace of scrolling a computer screen or using ctrl-f to locate a certain phrase. Holt cites Sven Birkeris’s argument that computers destroy the human capacity for deep reading—something that I’ve absolutely noticed in my own life when multitasking breaks my concentration.

Carr focuses on these negative aspects of technology, but his critics argue that he does not take enough time to weigh the positive effects computers and the internet have on the brain. The studies Holt mentions stress that running Google searches helps keep elderly minds sharp and that video games help children pay attention to multiple things at once. Obviously there is a tradeoff. Humans gain speed and efficiency while running the risk of losing the ability to concentrate deeply on one thing and to think creatively.

I remain undecided on the issue. Yes, it bothers me that the internet changes how we think and that it may encourage shallower thinking, but at the same time I think it is too extreme to say that the internet somehow robs us of our creativity. It is an individual choice to concentrate while reading or disable ones Facebook account. Hell, I’m even drafting this blurb on a legal pad so I’m not distracted by my email, but I wouldn’t go so far as to say it’s my email’s fault if I happen to glance at it.

The science and history involved in this whole debate are both fascinating. Neuroplasticity studies with little doubt confirm that our brains adapt to the way we feed them information. History tells the age-old story of intellectual resistance toward manuscripts, printing presses, photographs and typewriters—resistance that ultimately lost out when society changed. If the mind comes prepackaged with malleable characteristics that allow us to change how we think, then adapting to technology can’t be completely unnatural. And if we’re worried about losing the capacity to “deep think,” isn’t that concern something each person can combat on an individual level? Or is mob mentality too powerful to keep us from ignoring our computers and phones?

-- AnneFox - 28 Jun 2012

Navigation

Webs Webs

r4 - 22 Jan 2013 - 20:09:47 - IanSullivan
This site is powered by the TWiki collaboration platform.
All material on this collaboration platform is the property of the contributing authors.
All material marked as authored by Eben Moglen is available under the license terms CC-BY-SA version 4.
Syndicate this site RSSATOM